CHAPTER
I
INTRODUCTION
1.1 Background of Problem
At the heart of Vygotsky’s theory lies the understanding of human
cognition and learning as
social and cultural rather than individual phenomena. The theories of Vygotsky
are central to any serious discussion of children’s learning processes.
Vygotsky argued that children do not develop in isolation, rather that
learning takes place when the child is interacting with the social environment.
It is the responsibility of the teacher to establish an interactive instructional
situation in the classroom, where the child is an active learner and the teacher
uses their knowledge to guide learning. He
explored relationships between language and thought, instruction and development, everyday and academic
concept formation, and
a host of others.
Vygotsky
wrote in the Soviet Union during the 1920s and the 1930s. However, his work
published recently in the Western world in the 1960s. Since then, his writings
became very influential. Vygotsky’s research reveals
that, even so, heredity is not a sufficient condition and that a contribution
from the social environment in the form of a quite specific type of teaching
process is also needed.Vygotsky
was a Piage’s admirer. Although he agrees with Piaget that cognitive
development occurs gradually and is characterized by the style of thinking is
different, but Vygotsky disagreed with Piaget's view that children explore the
world alone and form a picture of his own reality.
In this paper will be explained more about Vygotsky’s
theory of cognitive development.
1.2 Formulation of Problem
1.
How
about Vygotsky’s concepts of cognitive development?
2.
How
about current perspectives on Vygotsky’s theory?
3.
How
the application of Vygotsky’s theory?
1.3 The Purposes
1.
To know about Vygotsky’s
concepts of cognitive development
2.
To know about current
perspectives on Vygotsky’s theory
3.
To
know the application of Vygotsky’s theory
CHAPTER II
DISCUSSION
2.1 Vygotsky’s Concept of Cognitive Development
Vygotsky believed that the adults in society foster
children’s cognitive development in an intentional and somewhat systematic
manner. They continually engage children in meaningful and challenging
activities and help them perform those activities successfully. Because
Vygotsky emphasized the importance of society and culture for promoting
cognitive growth, his theory is sometimes referred to as the sociocultural
perspective. The following major assumptions provide a summary of this
perspective:
a.
Genetic law of development
Every
function in the child’s cultural development appears twice: first, on the
social level, and later, on the individual level; first between people (interpsychological),
and then inside the child (intrapsychological). This applies equally to
voluntary attention, to logical memory, and to the formation of concepts. All
the higher functions originate as actual relations between human individuals. The
general genetic law of cultural development introduces the notion of some form
of relationship between something which is defined as social and something
which is defined as individual. This theory argued that social environment as
primer factor to formulate child’s cognitive development.
According
Vygotsky function of mental that higher in human being will appear and come
from their social life, while the function of intramental
formed through the acquisition and internalization of a person against the
social process, and meaning or construction of knowledge will be formed through
the internalization process. Firstly, child participated in particular social
activity without understand the meaning. Understanding will appear through
internalization process, but internalization here is transformative means that
give changing and developing[1].
Vygotsky saw change in mental functioning not as the
result of a linear process, but rather as the result of quantitative changes
leading to qualitative transformations. In these transformations, formerly
distinct processes became unified. Vygotsky grounded this approach in the
material world, starting his analysis with the changes that occurred when
humans began to control and use nature to meet their needs.[2]
b.
Zones of Proximal
Development
The
zone of proximal development of the child is the distance between his actual development,
determined with the help of independently solved tasks, and the level of
potential development of the child, determined with the help of tasks solved by
the child under the guidance of adults and in cooperation with his more intelligent
partners.[3]
The
concept of zone of proximal development as the theoretical attempt to
understand the operation of contradiction between internal possibilities and
external needs that constitutes the driving force of development. The concept
of Zone of Proximal Development was created by Vygotsky as a metaphor to assist
in explaining the way in which social and participatory learning takes place.
The
common conception of the zone of proximal development presupposes an
interaction on a task between a more competent person and a less competent
person, such that the less competent person becomes independently proficient at
what was initially a jointly accomplished task. Within this general conception,
three main aspects are often highlighted or emphasized (though not necessarily
all three by a single researcher). For the sake of discussion, these three
aspects together represent an ideal type that will be called the common
interpretation of the zone of proximal development. The first aspect
focuses on the idea that a person is able to perform a certain number of tasks
alone but in collaboration can perform a greater number of tasks. The second
aspect emphasizes how an adult/teacher/more competent person should interact
with a child. The third aspect focuses on properties of the learner, including
notions of a learner’s potential and/or readiness to learn.[4]
Moll
(1990) argued that a close reading of Mind in Society reveals the Development
of the ZPD concept from the method of double stimulation that emphasized the
child’s active engagement in finding new means to solve problems. Using words
to create a specific plan, the child achieves a much broader range of activity,
applying as tools not only those objects that lie near at hand, but searching
for and preparing such stimuli as can be useful in the solution of the task,
and planning future actions. (Vygotsky, 1978, p. 26)
The
search for a method of studying psychological change, which was arguably Vygotsky’s
central concern, was stated as follows: The search for method becomes one of
the most important problems of the entire enterprise of understanding the
uniquely human forms of psychological activity. In this case the method is
simultaneously prerequisite and product, the tool and result of the study. (Vygotsky,
1978, p. 65)
c.
Mediation
In
Vygotsky’s view, the conceptual isolation of consciousness and behavior that
had been initiated by the subjectivists and extended by the behaviorists had
led to a fundamental misconceptualization of both consciousness and behavior.
To achieve a truly unified psychological science, he insisted that a new system
of concepts and theories would have to be developed which would overcome the
conceptual isolation of behavior and consciousness that was so fundamental to
existing theoretical frameworks. Vygotsky’s explanation of the origin of the
higher mental functions included two components. First, he argued that the
higher mental functions rely on the mediation of behavior by signs and sign
systems, the most important of which is speech. Vygotsky saw signs as a special
type of stimuli that are used as “psychological tools,” tools that are
“directed towards the mastery or control of behavioral processes.[5]
Vygotsky
described psychological tools as devices for mastering mental processes. They
were seen as artificial and of social rather than organic or individual origin.
He gave the following examples of psychological tools: ‘language; various
systems for counting; mnemonic techniques; algebraic symbol systems; works of
art; writing; schemes, diagrams, maps and mechanical drawings; all sorts of
conventional signs (Vygotsky, 1960/1981, pp. 136–7).
The
very idea of mediation carries with it a number of significant implications
concerning pedagogic control. In that the concept denies the possibility of
total determinism through external forces it is associated with an intellectual
baggage which is potentially highly charged, especially in the political
context in which these ideas were originally promulgated. In the extract
reprinted below it is clear that Vygotsky was arguing that humans master
themselves through external symbolic, cultural systems rather than being
subjugated by and in them. Vygotsky (1978) discussed a number of examples of
early psychological tools such as tying knots. He suggested that when a knot is
tied in a handkerchief as a reminder it acts as an external aspect of
remembering.
The
process of memorizing is reconstructed through the transformation of an
external object as a reminder of something. He contrasts the act of memorizing
before and after the knot is used as a psychological tool. Vygotsky also argues
that the ways in which tools and signs are used vary as function of context and
the child’s own development. He refers to the ‘natural history of the sign’ as
a way of emphasizing that, in the course of development, the psychological
function that may be fulfilled by signs may also develop and change. Thus,
speech may have several functions (e.g. labeling and planning). In the early
stages of development, Vygotsky suggests that speech may assume an important
labeling function in that it enables the child to identify a particular object,
to single it out and distinguish it from others. He argues that increasing
sophistication in the use of language allows the child to become progressively
more independent of the sensory field (Vygotsky, 1978, p. 32).
According to practice, in C. Asri Budiningsih,
(2005: 103), that there are two types of mediation:
·
Metacognitive mediation of interpersonal communication to use a tool for self
regulation (self regulation), which includes: self-planning, self-monitoring,
self checking, and self-Evaluating. Older adults or peers who are competent to
use these tools to help children, then children internalize the tools for self
regulation.
·
Cognitive mediation, namely the use of cognitive tools (in the form of
spontaneous concepts or scientific concepts) to solve the problems associated
with certain knowledge (subject-domain problem). More assured they were correct
scientific concept of the concept of spontaneous, in which the concept is
internalized students (as mediator) in solving the problem. Scientific concepts
can be either declarative knowledge (inadequate) and procedural knowledge in
the form of method or strategy that is more adequate to solve the problem.
Solving process more meaningful for the way he is combine the concepts and
procedures through demonstration and practice.
2.2
Current Perspectives on Vygotsky’s Theory
Vygotsky
focused more on the processes through which children develop than on the
characteristics that children of particular ages are likely to exhibit.
Furthermore, his descriptions of such processes were often imprecise and
lacking in detail. Nevertheless, many contemporary theorist and practitioners
have made considerable use of Vygotsky’s idea.[6]
a.
Guided Participation
Older family members often allow
young children to perform household tasks (cooking, cleaning, pinting, and so
on) while providing guidance about how to do these tasks appropriately.
Teachers, too, often introduce students to adult tasks within a structured and
supportive context. For instance, they might ask students to conduct laboratory
experiments, write letter to government officials, or search the internet for
specific information, while always providing the support the students need to
accomplish such tasks successfully. When we assist our student as they perform
adultlike activities, we engage them in guided participation in the world of
adults (Radziszewa & Rogoff, 1990, 1991) As we guide them, we should also
use some of the language that adults frequently use in such context; for
example, when students conduct scientific experiments, we should use words such
as hypothesis, evidence, and theory as we help them evaluate their procedures
and results (Perkins, 1992)
b.
Scaffolding
Theorists have given considerable
thought to the kinds of assistance that can help children complete challenging
tasks. The term of scaffolding is often used here. Adults and other more
competent individuals provide some form of guidance or structure that enables
children to perform tasks in their zone of proximal development. In much the
same way, and adult guiding a child through a new task may provide an initial
scaffold to support the child’s early efforts. As teacher we can provide
variety of support mechanism to help students master tasks; here are some
examples:
·
Work with students to develop a
plan for dealing with a new task
·
Demonstrate the proper
performance of the tasks in a way that students can easily imitate
·
Divide a complex task into
several smaller, simple task
·
Provide structure or guidelines
about how the task should be accomplished
·
Provide a calculator, computer
software or other technology that makes some aspects of their task easier
·
Ask questions that get student
thinking in appropriate ways about the task
·
Keep student’s attention
focused on the relevant aspects of the task
·
Keep student’s motivation to
complete the task
·
Remind students what their
goals is in performing the task
·
Give frequent feedback about
how students are progressing
As students develop increasing
competence, we can gradually withdraw some of these support mechanism,
eventually allowing students to perform the task independently. In a manner of
speaking, when we remove such scaffolding, we allow and encourage students to
stand on their own two feet.
c.
Apprenticeships
The cognitive apprenticeship
approach proposes that learners should engage in meaningful learning and
problem solving whilst working with authentic problems. In an apprenticeship a
learner works intensively with an expert to accomplish complex tasks that he or
she cannot do independently. The expert provides considerable structure and
guidance throughout the process, gradually removing scaffolding and giving the
learner more responsibility as competence increases. Throughout an
apprenticeship a student often learns not only how to perform a task but also
how to think about a task; such is sometimes called a cognitive apprenticeship.
Although apprenticeships can differ widely from one context to another, they
typically have some or all of these features:
·
Modeling. The teacher carries out the task,
simultaneously thinking aloud about the process, while the student observes and
listens
·
Coaching. As the student performs the task,
the teacher gives frequent suggestions, hints, and feedback
·
Scaffolding. The teacher provides various
forms of support for the student, perhaps by simplifying the task, breaking it
into smaller and more manageable components or providing less complicated
equipment
·
Articulation. The student explains what he or
she is doing and why, allowing the teacher to examine the student’s knowledge,
reasoning, and problem solving strategies
·
Reflection. The teacher asks the student to
compare his or her performance with that of experts, or perhaps with an ideal
model of how the task should be done
·
Increasing complexity and diversity of tasks.
As the student gains greater proficiency, the teacher presents more complex,
challenging, and varied tasks to complete
·
Exploration. The teacher encourages the
student to frame questions and problems on his or her own and thereby to expand
and refine acquired skills
Apprenticeships are clearly labor
intensive as such their use in the classroom is not always practical or
logistically feasible. Apprenticeships frequently take place in natural
settings for example in a studio, workshop, or place of employment and involve
real life tasks.
d.
Peer Interaction
Peer interaction is a term that
embodies the concept of students as active learners and participants in all
aspects of schooling. It recognizes that students represent a significant
resource that often is untapped in schools today. Peer interaction involves a
variety of practices and strategies that utilize students as instructors,
advocates, and decision makers (Snell & Janey, 2000; Villa &
Thousand,1996). The benefits of peer involvement in these areas have been
well-documented as follows:
- The use of peer tutor and partner learning systems have been demonstrated to be a cost-effective way for teachers to increase the amount of individualized instructional attention available to their students (Armstrong, Stahlbrand, Conlon, & Pierson, 1979; Villa & Thousand, 1988).
- Peers as instructors have been noted to be more effective than adults because they tend to use more age-appropriate vocabulary and examples, are more directive than adults, and are more familiar with potential frustrations (Good & Brophy, 1987; Thousand & McNeil, 1990).
- Effective peer support systems do not occur spontaneously or in isolation. As with any effective strategy, active planning, support, and facilitation are required to maximize this resource.
- The outcomes of successful peer collaboration can be social as well as academic. Peer collaboration builds relationships between students and results in a more caring community of learners.
This
section provides an overview of strategies and approaches that can be used to
facilitate peer collaboration and promote positive classroom environments.
2.3
The Application of Vygotsky’s Theory
The concept of Vygotsky about reconstruction
of knowledge in social setting if it is applied in learning context, a teacher
should pay attention about those things. The teacher’s
task is twofold. First, in the history lesson, the teacher stimulates the
students to connect their knowledge, partly acquired beyond. Secondary
education, with the academic concepts to be learned formally at school. And,
second, the teacher has to prevent an academic concept from remaining an empty
shell for the student, that is, a concept that is not experienced or understood
and can only be learned by rote. Experience teaches us that such undigested
knowledge is of no use to students and rapidly evaporates. Teachers must learn
how to guide their students in sequences of assignments aiming at using
everyday knowledge as a means to absorb and own academic concepts. In our
teacher education courses, we devote ample discussion time to these tasks of
the history teacher. Basic to these tasks is a learning-psychological starting
point, combined with our interpretation of historical consciousness. A teacher should provide some kinds and level
of helps that can give facility the student in order they can solve some
problems that is faced by them. In the word of cognitive psychology, these helps is known as cognitive scaffolding.
Those helps can be described in the form of giving examples, guidance or
orientation of doing, program or space, the steps or procedures of doing
assignment etc.
The guidance or
help from the adult person or a friend who more competent and very effective to
increase the productivities of learning. Those helps must suitable with the
context of sociocultural or student’s characteristic. The guidance by adult
person or peer who is more competent benefit to understand the semiotic tool
like language, sign and symbols. The student or children experience the
internalization process and then these tools has function as mediator for the
next psychology processes of the student themselves. So, the forms learning of
cooperative-collaborative and the learning of contextual is very appropriate applied.
By the conception
of the readiness to learn, so the understanding about the characteristic of
student that relate with sociocultural and the first capability as footing in
learning is needed more to look the articulation, so it can be produced soft
equipment of learning that really challenge but it is still productive and
creative
CHAPTER III
CLOSING
3.1
Conclusion
Vygotsky’s basic assumptions are emphasized the
importance of society and culture for promoting cognitive growth, his theory is
sometimes referred to as the sociocultural perspective. The following major
assumptions provide a summary of some perspectives such as: genetic law of
development, zone of proximal development, and mediation.
Current
perspectives on Vygotsky’s theory focused more on the processes through which
children develop than on the characteristics that children of particular ages
are likely to exhibit. Furthermore, his descriptions of such processes were
often imprecise and lacking in detail. Nevertheless, many contemporary theorist
and practitioners have made considerable use of Vygotsky’s idea such as: guided
participation, scaffolding, apprenticeships, and peer interaction.
The application of
Vygotsky’s theory in
social setting if it is applied in learning context, a teacher should pay
attention about those things. In every planning and implementation in learning
the attention of teacher must be concentrated to the group of student who can
not solve some problems of learning by themselves that they who only can solve
problems of learning with help. A teacher should provide some kinds and level
of helps that can give facility the student in order they can solve some
problems that is faced by them.
3.2
Suggestion
From some explanations about Vygotsky’s theory of cognitive
development, the compiler suggests to all of readers in order they can know
about the basic assumptions and also the application of Vygotsky’s theory. The
compiler also suggests to all of readers in order use this paper as good
reference in the future.
BIBLIOGRAPHY
Kozulin,
Alex, 2003, Vygotsky’s Educational Theory in Cultural
Context, United States of America: Cambridge University Press
Budiningsih, Asri, 2005, Belajar
dan Pembelajaran, Bandung: Rineka Cipta
M. Reynolds
W, 2003, Educational Psychology,
Canada
Shayer, Michael, 2003, Not just Piaget; not just Vygotsky, and certainly not Vygotsky as alternative to Piaget, King’s College, University of London
Daniels Harry, 1996, An Introduction to Vygotsky, USA
Ellis, Ormfod, 2003, Educational Psychology, Colombus
[1] C.Asri Budiningsih, Belajar dan Pembelajaran, p.100
[2] William M. Reynolds, Educational
Psychology, p.152
[3] Michael Shayer, Not just Piaget;
not just Vygotsky, and certainly not Vygotsky as alternative to Piaget,
p.470
[4] Alex Kozulin, Vygotsky’s
Educational Theory in Cultural Context, p.41
[5] Harry Daniels, An Introduction to Vygotsky, p.30
[6] Jeanne Ellis Ormfod, Educational Psychology, p.39
Tidak ada komentar:
Posting Komentar